homebutton

European perch

Perca fluviatilis

Perca fluviatilis (European perch)
enlarge button
Distribution
Distribution map: Perca fluviatilis (European perch)

least concern



Information


Authors: Jenny Volstorf, João L. Saraiva
Version: B | 1.1 (2020-01-18)


Reviewer: Kerstin Glaus
Editor: Billo Heinzpeter Studer

Initial release: 2016-09-15
Version information:
  • Appearance: B
  • Last minor update: 2020-01-18

Cite as: »Volstorf, Jenny, and João L. Saraiva. 2020. Perca fluviatilis (Dossier). In: fair-fish database, ed. fair-fish. World Wide Web electronic publication. Version B | 1.1. https://fair-fish-database.net.«





1  Remarks

1.1 General remarks

No data found yet.

1.2 Other remarks

No data found yet.


2  Ethograms

In the wild: on feeding, daily rhythm, social behaviour 
  • For feeding 1.
  • For daily rhythm 2 3.
  • For schooling, shoaling 2 4.
In the farm or lab: on reproduction, social behaviour, cognitive abilities 
  • For courtship and spawning 5.
  • For dominance, subordination, and aggression 6.
  • For learning 7 8.



3  Distribution

Species occurrence (natural and introduced). Note: areas either verified by FAO records ("good" point) or not 9.

Natural distribution: Europe, inland and coastal waters 
  • Observations inland waters continental Europe: lake of Robertville, Belgium 10, lake Varese, north-west Italy 11, lake Wallersee, Austria 12, Orlik reservoir, Czech Republic 4, Rimov reservoir, Czech Republic 13 14, river Meuse, Belgium 15, Slapy reservoir, Czech Republic 4.
  • Observations inland waters England: loch Kinord and loch Davan, Scotland 5, lake Windermere, north-west England 3.
  • Observations inland waters Scandinavia: lake Abborrtjärn 3, Sweden 16, lake Ängersjön, Sweden 17 18 19, lake Bjännsjön, Sweden 17 18, lake Fisksjön, Sweden 17 18 19, lake Harkkojärvi 1 20, lakes Kinnasjärvi and Koppelojärvi 20, lake Nimetön, Finland 1, lake Stöcksjön, Sweden 17 18, lake Trehörningen, Sweden 17 18, lake Tuopanjärvi, Finland 20, lake Ulvenvannet, Norway 18.
  • Observations coastal waters: off Norrbyn, Gulf of Bothnia, Sweden 18.
Introduced: inland waters worldwide 
  • Observations continental Europe: lake Chabarovice, Czech Republic 13.
  • Observations Oceania: lake Pounui, New Zealand 2.



4  Natural co-existence

Natural co-existence: Bass, Brown trout, Bullhead catfish, Carp, Eel, Peled, Tench (excluding predators and prey of P. fluviatilis) 
  • Observations Bass WILD: Micropterus salmoides: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.
  • Observations Brown trout WILD: Salmo trutta: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2.
  • Observations Bullhead catfish WILD: Ictalurus melas: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.
  • Observations Carp WILD: Cyprinus carpio, Carassius carassius: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.
  • Observations Eel WILD: Anguilla australis Richardson and A. dieffenbachii Gray: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2, Anguilla anguilla: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.
  • Observations Peled WILD: Coregonus peled: forest lakes Horkkajärvi and Nimetön, southern Finland 1.
  • Observations Tench WILD: Tinca tinca: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.



5  Substrate and/or shelter

5.1 Substrate

Substrate range, substrate preference: opportunistic – reported from lakes with plants as well as reservoirs with sandy, muddy, and rocky bottoms 
  • Plants:
    • WILD: JUVENILES-ADULTS were observed with floating Sphagnum peat moss: forest lake Nimetön, southern Finland 1.
    • WILD: JUVENILES-ADULTS were observed with Carex rostrata, Menyanthes trifoliata, Lobelia dortmanna, Nymphaea alba: lake Abborrtjärn 3, Sweden 16.
    • For substrate and spawning D1.
  • Rocks and stones:
    • WILD: JUVENILES-ADULTS were found over rocky bays of rocks and rubble: Rimov reservoir, Czech Republic 13.
  • Sand and mud:
    • WILD: JUVENILES-ADULTS were found over sandy and muddy beaches: Rimov reservoir, Czech Republic 13.
  • Other substrate: no data found yet.

5.2 Shelter or cover

Shelter or cover preference: uses shelter during establishing hierarchy (further research needed) 
  • Plants: no data found yet.
  • Rocks and stones: no data found yet.
  • Sand and mud: no data found yet.
  • Other cover: no data found yet.
  • For shelter and establishing hierarchy D2.
Shelter or cover and stress: enrichment increases number of attacks and chases (further research needed) 
  • Environmental enrichment and aggression:
    • LAB: in groups of three 0+ JUVENILES in 10 L aquaria with grey plastic tube (3 cm diameter, 8 cm length, open on one side) at bottom, dominant and rival JUVENILES mainly attacked and chased each other. Less often: bites, displays. No aggression in control aquaria without tube, no aggression during feeding 6.



6  Food, foraging, hunting, feeding

6.1 Trophic level and general considerations on food needs

Trophic level: 4.4 
  • Observations: 4.4±0.0 se 21.
Impacts of feed fishery: contributes to overfishing, challenges animal welfare 
  • Carnivorous D3. The fishery that provides fish meal and fish oil has two major impacts:
    1. It contributes considerably to overfishing, as it accounts for 1/4 22 or even 1/3 23 of the world catch volume.
    2. It challenges animal welfare, because in the face of 450-1,000 MILLIARD wild fishes caught worldwide each year to be processed into fish meal or fish oil 24, the individual fish gets overlooked and, thus, suffering increases at rearing, live marketing, and slaughtering levels 25.

6.2 Food items

Food items, food preference: carnivorous, increasing prey size with increasing age 
  • Food items: carnivorous:
    • WILD, JUVENILES-ADULTS: mainly Zooplankton, crustacean Zooplankton (Asellus aquaticus), insect larvae (Ephemeroptera, Trichoptera, Chironomidae, Odonata), insects (Heteroptera): forest lakes Horkkajärvi and Nimetön, southern Finland 1.
    • WILD, JUVENILES-ADULTS: widely-varied diet, depending on season: mainly insect larvae or pupae (Chaoboridae, Chironomidae) with peak in April, crustacean Zooplankton (Cladocera, Daphnia sp., Leptodora kindtii, copepods) with peak in April, fish (Scardinius erythrophthalmus, Rutilus rutilus) and crayfish Orconectes limosus with peaks in July and September: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.
    • For cannibalism D4.
  • Food items and habitat: no data found yet.
  • Food items and life stages: mainly Zooplankton as JUVENILES, increasing proportion of fish with increasing age:
    • WILD: lake Abborrtjärn 3, Sweden 16:
      0+ JUVENILES: mainly Zooplankton, chironomids, macroinvertebrates,
      1+ JUVENILES: mainly macroinvertebrates, zooplankton and chironomids.
    • WILD: forest lake Horkkajärvi, southern Finland 1:
      JUVENILES of 7-9.9 cm: mainly Zooplankton,
      JUVENILES of 10-12.9 cm: mainly Zooplankton, crustacean Zooplankton (Asellus aquaticus), and insect larvae (Trichoptera, Chironomidae),
      ADULTS of 13-15.9 cm: mainly crustacean Zooplankton (Asellus aquaticus), insects (Heteroptera), and insect larvae (Odonata, Trichoptera).
    • WILD: forest lake Nimetön, southern Finland 1:
      JUVENILES of 7-9.9 cm: mainly Zooplankton,
      JUVENILES of 10-12.9 cm: mainly insect larvae (Ephemeroptera, Odonata, Chironomidae),
      ADULTS of 13-15.9 cm: mainly Zooplankton, insect larvae (Ephemeroptera, Chironomidae), and crustacean Zooplankton (Asellus aquaticus),
      ADULTS of 16->19 cm: mainly insects (Heteroptera) and insect larvae (Odonata).
    • WILD: three diet shifts in lake Varese, north-west Italy 11:
      JUVENILES of 9-12.5 cm: mainly crustacean Zooplankton (65%) and insect larvae/pupae (35%),
      ADULTS of 12.5-16 cm: mainly insect larvae/pupae and crayfish (63%) and crustacean Zooplankton (37%),
      ADULTS of 16-18 cm: mainly insect larvae/pupae (65%), fish (22%), crustacean Zooplankton (13%),
      ADULTS >18 cm: mainly fish (55%).
    • FARM: in natural ponds 28:
      Until 20 days post hatch: Zooplankton Daphnia, rotifers (Keratella, Asplancha), copepods, nauplius larvae,
      25-40 days post hatch: slight size bimodality with a) larger (3-7 cm difference) FRY preying on Abramis brama larvae and smaller FRY, macroinvertebrates (ephemeropterans, odonates, chironomids), b) smaller FRY sticking mainly to Zooplankton,
      >40 days post hatch: a) larger 0+ JUVENILES: mainly fish (P. fluviatilis, Abramis brama), macroinvertebrates, b) smaller 0+ JUVENILES: mainly macroinvertebrates, Zooplankton.
  • Food preference: no data found yet.
  • Food partitioning: no data found yet.
  • Prey density: no data found yet.
  • Prey size selectivity: no data found yet.
  • Particle size: no data found yet.

6.3 Feeding behaviour

Feeding style, foraging mode: littoral 
  • WILD: JUVENILES-ADULTS searched for food in littoral zone, snipping for food items one by one: forest lakes Horkkajärvi and Nimetön, southern Finland 1.
  • For foraging and vision D5.
Effects on feeding: direct relation with temperature 
  • For feeding and low temperatures and disturbance D6.
For feeding and dominance D7.



7  Photoperiod

7.1 Daily rhythm

Daily rhythm: crepuscular (further research needed) 
  • Daily rhythm:
    • WILD: during night, little activity, single JUVENILES-ADULTS rested on bottom in open water: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2.
    • WILD, ADULTS: major activity peak at dusk ca 18:00 h and smaller peak at dawn ca 06:00 h in October-April with high surface visual irradiance and clear water during the day. Several peaks throughout photophase in May-September with lower light intensities during the day due to reduced surface visual irradiance and algal bloom. Observations indicate that ADULTS avoid high light intensities: lake Windermere, north-west England 3.
  • Nocturnal activity: Daily rhythm.
  • Phototaxis: no data found yet.
Photoperiod and stress: nocturnal lighting influences circadian melatonin rhythm but not stress (further research needed) 
  • LAB: at density of 5.1 kg/m3, JUVENILES in 80 x 30 x 35 cm aquaria taped with black foil on all sides and subjected to artificial illumination: 7,000 lux during 14 h daytime including 3 h dusk or dawn transition, 10 h nighttime with either 1, 10, or 100 lux (control: 0 lux at nighttime). After 10 days, reduced mean melatonin concentration in tank water under 10 and 100 lux compared to control (ca 0.8-1.5 pg/L/kg versus ca 6-9 pg/L/kg); 1 lux in between (ca 1.8-2.5 pg/L/kg). Circadian rhythm of melatonin production with increase after dusk and decrease after dawn under 1 lux but not under 10 or 100 lux. Lower median cortisol concentration in tank water under 100 lux compared to 1, 10 lux, and control (ca 3-4.5 ng/L/kg versus ca 4-6 ng/L/kg). Results indicate that light influences circadian melatonin rhythm, but nocturnal lighting does not influence stress 29.

7.2 Light intensity

No data found yet.

7.3 Light colour

No data found yet.


8  Water parameters

8.1 Water temperature

Standard temperature range, temperature preference: 6.1-28 °C 
  • Standard temperature range: 6.1-28 °C:
    • Observations WILD, FRY: average 12.1-20.4 °C: Rimov reservoir, Czech Republic 14.
    • Observations WILD, JUVENILES-ADULTS: 6.1-19.5 °C: lake Windermere, north-west England 3, surface temperature 6.7-24.2 °C: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2, 22-28 °C in epilimnetic layer, 9-11 °C in hypolimnetic layer: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.
  • Temperature preference: no data found yet.
  • For temperature and...
    ...depth D8,
    ...spawning D1.
Temperature and growth: must exceed 13-14 °C (further research needed) 
  • Temperature must exceed: 13-14 °C:
    • WILD, JUVENILES: growth started with water temperatures 13-14 °C: lake Windermere, north-west England 30.
  • Temperature must not go beyond: no data found yet.
  • Optimal temperature for growth: no data found yet.
  • For temperature and feeding D6.

8.2 Oxygen

Dissolved oxygen range: <10-150%, 6-9 mg/L 
  • Observations WILD, FRY: ca 6-7 mg/L at 7-9 m, ca 8-9 mg/L at 0-2 m: Rimov reservoir, Czech Republic 14.
  • Observations WILD, JUVENILES-ADULTS: 100-150% in epilimnetic layer, <10% in hypolimnetic layer: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.

8.3 Salinity

Salinity tolerance, standard salinity range: probably euryhaline (further research needed) 
  • Salinity tolerance:
    • Natural and introduced distribution in fresh water or brackish water at most D9 D10.
  • Standard salinity range: no data found yet.

8.4 pH

No data found yet.

8.5 Turbidity

Standard turbidity range: Secchi depth 1.1-7.5 m 
  • Standard turbidity range: no data found yet.
  • Secchi depth (water transparency): 1.1-7.5 m:
    • Observations WILD, LARVAE-JUVENILES: 1.1-3.8 m: Orlik reservoir, Czech Republic, 6-7.5 m: Slapy reservoir, Czech Republic 4.
    • Observations WILD, JUVENILES-ADULTS: 3 m: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.
    • Observations WILD, ADULTS: 2.3 m: Rimov reservoir, Czech Republic, 6.8 m: lake Chabrovice, Czech Republic (introduced) 13.
  • For Secchi depth and spawning D1.

8.6 Water hardness

No data found yet.

8.7 NO4

No data found yet.

8.8 Other

No data found yet.


9  Swimming

9.1 Swimming type, swimming mode

Swimming type, swimming mode: sub-carangiform 
  • Observations: 31.

9.2 Swimming speed

No data found yet.

9.3 Home range

No data found yet.

9.4 Depth

Depth range, depth preference: 0-18.5 m, moves deeper with decreasing temperatures 
  • Depth range in the wild: 0-18.5 m:
    • Observations WILD, LARVAE: 0-2 m: lake Abborrtjärn 3, Sweden 16.
    • Observations WILD, FRY: maximum 1 m (littoral), 0-2 m (epipelagic), 7-9 m (bathypelagic): Rimov Reservoir, Czech Republic 14.
    • Observations WILD, LARVAE-JUVENILES: 4.5-18.5 m: Orik reservoir, Czech Republic 4.
    • Observations WILD, JUVENILES-ADULTS: schools observed in 0.5-2 m depth: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2, 2-6 m: lake Abborrtjärn 3, Sweden 16.
    • Observations WILD, ADULTS: 1-12 m: lake Windermere, north-west England 3, 1.5 m: channel in Norden, Germany 8.
    • For depth and spawning D1.
  • Depth in cages or tanks: no data found yet.
  • Depth preference: no data found yet.
  • Depth and daily rhythm:
    • WILD: during summer, ADULTS moved deeper to 1.5-3 m in early evening to feed on JUVENILES: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2.
  • Depth and low temperatures:
    • WILD: annual catch peak in 1-12 m in August at 19.5 °C, lowest catch in January at 6.1 °C. Majority of catch in 1-6 m depth during July-August, majority of catch in 4-12 m rest of the year: lake Windermere, north-west England 3.
    • WILD: JUVENILES-ADULTS moved deeper (seldomly >6 m) in autumn at surface temperature ca 16 °C: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2.
  • Depth and high temperatures: no data found yet.
  • Position in habitat and age: no data found yet.
  • Depth and light intensity: no data found yet.
  • Depth and noise: no data found yet.
  • Depth and threat: no data found yet.

9.5 Migration

No data found yet.


10  Growth

10.1 Ontogenetic development

Mature egg: 0.9-1.6 mm diameter, average 8.7 mg 
  • Observations time from fertilisation until hatching: no data found yet.
  • Observations size WILD: 0.9-1.4 mm diameter: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2, 0.9-1.6 mm: Lochs Davan and Kinord, Scotland 5.
  • Observations weight WILD: average 8.7 mg: lake of Robertville, Belgium 10.
Larvae: hatching to 15-17 days, 4.8-6.5 mm, average 0.9-1.0 mg 
  • Observations age at yolk sac absorption and mouth opening LAB: days 15-17 32.
  • Observations TOTAL LENGTH WILD: 5.2-6.5 mm fork length after hatching: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2.
  • Observations TOTAL LENGTH and weight LAB: 4.8-5.3 mm after hatching 32, mean 0.9 mg after hatching 10, mean 6.5 mm, 1 mg after hatching 33.
Fry: beginning of exogenous feeding, 4-34 days, 9-23 mm, 18-22 mg 
  • Observations age at beginning of exogenous feeding LAB: day 4-19 3532.
  • Observations age and TOTAL LENGTH WILD: 16-34 days, range 9-23 mm, average 13.7-19.3 mm: Rimov reservoir, Czech Republic 14.
  • Observations age and weight LAB: 10 days, 18-22 mg 15.
Juveniles, sexual maturity: fully developed (ca 2 months) to beginning of maturity (0-4 years), 6.4-19.9 cm, 9-126 g 
  • Observations age: D11.
  • Observations TOTAL LENGTH WILD: 9-12.5 cm: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.
  • Observations weight FARM: 57.8 g 36.
  • Observations age and weight LAB: average 39.6 g 37, average 59.6 g 38, 63.5-78.6 g 39, day 77, ca 5-6 g: early gametogenesis in males and females 15.
  • Sexual maturity: males: 0-2 years, 10.2-13.7 cm or less, 9-18 g, females: 1-4 years, 10-19.9 cm, 23-126 g:
    • Observations age, TOTAL LENGTH, and weight of males WILD: 2 years, 10.2-13.7 cm, 9-18 g: lake Windermere, north-west England 30, during first year: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2, mainly 2 years and 6 cm fork length: Loch Kinord and Loch Davan, Scotland 5, 2 years and 8-10 cm: forest lake Horkkajärvi, southern Finland 1, from age 0+ on, 73% at age 1+: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.
    • Observations age, TOTAL LENGTH, and weight of females WILD: 3-4 years, 13.4-19.9 cm, 23-126 g: lake Windermere, north-west England 30, during second year: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2, mainly 4 years and 15 cm fork length: Loch Kinord, Scotland, from 3 years and 17-18 cm fork length on: Loch Davan, Scotland 5, 3 years and 10-11 cm: forest lake Horkkajärvi, southern Finland 1, 12-13 cm: forest lake Nimetön, southern Finland 1, from age 1+ on, 76% at age 2+: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.
Adults: 4-15 years, 10-34+ cm, 22-880 g 
  • Observations age, TOTAL LENGTH, and weigth WILD: females: 12-15 years, 20.4-25.1 cm, 115-166 g, males: 10 years, 15.8-22.7 cm, 105-132 g: lake Windermere, north-west England 30, females 19.7-34.3 cm (mean 25.9 cm), males 15.1-27.3 cm (mean 23.8 cm): lake Windermere, north-west England 3, male: 6 years and 26.2 cm fork length, 8 years and 21.7 cm fork length, female: 10 years and 42.4 cm fork length, 12 years and 30.9 cm fork length: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2, 89.7-129.7 g at age 4, 367.3-880.0 g at age 6-15: Loch Kinord and Loch Davan, Scotland 5, 22 g at 6 years, 13-16 cm at age 8+ (stunted): forest lake Horkkajärvi, southern Finland 1, 62 g at 6 years: forest lake Nimetön, southern Finland 1, 10-12 cm: channel in Norden, Germany 8, ca 27 cm at age 7+ years: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.
  • Observations TOTAL LENGTH FARM: 19-33 cm 28.

10.2 Sexual conversion

No data found yet.

10.3 Sex ratio

No data found yet.

10.4 Effects on growth

Growth rate: 5-7 cm/year in first year, 1-2 cm/year from fifth year on 
  • Growth:
    • WILD: growth halted during band formation on opercular bones in winter and early spring: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2.
  • Natural growth rate:
    • WILD, JUVENILES: estimated growth rates of ca 5 cm in fourth and fifth year coincided with cannibalism: lake Äbborrtjärn 3, Sweden 16.
    • WILD, JUVENILES-ADULTS: growth rates estimated at ca 7 cm in first year, 4-5 cm in second year, 1-3 cm in third and fourth year, thereafter ca 1-2 cm/year: lake Windermere, north-west England 30.
    • WILD, JUVENILES-ADULTS: growth rates estimated at ca 5 cm in first year, 3-4 cm in second year, 2-3 cm in third and fourth year, thereafter ca 1-2 cm/year: forest lakes Horkkajärvi and Nimetön, southern Finland 1, lake Abbörrtjärn 3, Sweden 16.
Growth and sex: bimodal pattern, noticeable from the second or third year on 
  • Observations bimodal pattern WILD, JUVENILES-ADULTS: range of females 10.5-29.4 cm (mean 20 cm), males 10-27.5 cm (mean 17.7 cm), mean total body mass for females 112 g, males 71 g: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.
  • Beginning of noticeable size difference:
    • WILD, JUVENILES: females grew faster than males after the first year: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2.
    • WILD, JUVENILES-ADULTS: similar growth of males and females in first two years, then mature individuals larger than immature individuals, mature females grew faster than mature males: lake Windermere, north-west England 30.
    • WILD, JUVENILES-ADULTS: faster growth of males in first two years, from then on faster growth of females: forest lakes Horkkajärvi and Nimetön, southern Finland 1.
For growth and...
...water temperature D12,
...water temperature and disturbance D6,
...tank colour D5.


10.5 Deformities and malformations

No data found yet.


11  Reproduction

11.1 Nest building

Nest building: none 
  • Nest building and substrate: no data found yet.
  • Nest building and water velocity: no data found yet.
  • Nest building and water depth: no data found yet.
  • Nest building: no data found yet.
  • For breeding type D13.

11.2 Attraction, courtship, mating

Courtship sequence: female encircles water plants for ca 30 min, increasing in speed (further research needed) 
  • Courtship sequence:
    • LAB, ADULTS: in aquarium, female with gravid belly attracted 4-5 males. One male followed while she circled water plants, frequently changed directions, often spiralled upward and paused in between. Male stayed behind at 1-2 body lengths, sometimes prodding the female's vent. Remaining males in attendance stared at vent, followed if female changed site and replaced male if broke off. No aggression recorded. Female circled for 30 min, increasing in speed 5.
  • Courtship duration: 30 min Courtship sequence.

11.3 Spawning

Mating system: polyandry (further research needed) 
  • Polyandry:
    • Observations LAB, ADULTS: 5.
Spawning conditions: gravel or submerged vegetation, winter-spring, 11-22 °C, 0-12 m depth 
  • Spawning substrate: gravel, macrophytes, tree roots, branches:
    • Observations WILD, ADULTS: macrophytes in 2-3 m depth: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2, wide variety including gravel, aquatic macrophytes, tree roots, dead branches, and artificial substrate composed of intermeshed hornbeam Carpinus betulus and hazel Corylus avellana branches positioned at bottom in 2-8 m depth: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.
    • WILD, ADULTS: more egg strands found in grassy than rocky bays or main reservoir (ca 3-5 strands/100 m versus 0.1-0.5 strands/100 m), mostly on reed canarygrass but also goat willow Salix caprea, logged trees, branches, raspberry Rubus ideaus, blackberry Rubus fruticosus, dog rose Rosa canina, common rush Juncus effusus: Rimov reservoir, Czech Republic 13.
  • Spawning season: winter-spring:
    • Observations WILD, ADULTS: winter, early spring: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2, April: river Sunoga 32, pre-spawning females at late-developing and gravid stage were caught in April and May at 11-22 °C, eggs were caught in April and May: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11, mid April-beginning of May, 14-16 °C: Rimov reservoir, Czech Republic 13.
  • Spawning (day)time: no data found yet.
  • Spawning temperature: Spawning season.
  • Spawning salinity: no data found yet.
  • Spawning and water velocity: no data found yet.
  • Spawning depth: 0-12 m depending on waves, submerged vegetation, and Secchi depth:
    • WILD, ADULTS: Rimov reservoir (Czech Republic) little vulnerable to wind and thus waves through surrounding forests and meanders, flooded vegetation in littoral zone, Secchi depth 2.3 m: 98.1% of 156 egg strands in <0.5 m, in grassy bays mostly 0-0.1 m. Deeper deposition (0.5-1 m) in observation year with low water level and missing submerged vegetation. Lake Chabrovice (Czech Republic) exposed to higher wind velocities because no surrounding forest, abundant submerged vegetation partly to deepest depth, Secchi depth 6.8 m: 90.5% of egg strands in >3 m, 95% in <12 m, none in <1 m 13.
  • Spawning density: no data found yet.
Male:female ratio resulting in spawning, composition of the broodstock: 1:4.6 (further research needed) 
  • Male:female ratio resulting in spawning:
    • Observations WILD, ADULTS: 1:4.6: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2.
  • Composition of broodstock: no data found yet.
Spawning sequence: female releases single egg strand, several males release milt simultaneously (further research needed) 
  • Spawning sequence:
    • WILD, ADULTS: males left spawning grounds immediately after spawning: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2.
    • LAB, ADULTS: in aquarium, female rapidly spiralled clockwise through water plants for 5 s while releasing single egg strand. At least two of 4-5 males in attendance simultaneously released milt 5.
  • Spawning duration: Spawning sequence.

11.4 Fecundity

Female fecundity: 33,000-146,000 eggs/kg body weight 
  • Number of spawns: no data found yet.
  • Fecundity per spawn:
    • Observations absolute fecundity WILD, ADULTS: 2,657-63,858 eggs at 14.6-42.2 cm fork length: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2, 9,277-18,872 at age 4, 34,168-74,124 at age 6-15: Loch Kinord and Loch Davan, Scotland 5, mean 5,038 at age 2+ and 17.5-19.5 cm, mean 10,913 at age 3+ and 20-22 cm, mean 18,551 at age 4+ and 23-25 cm: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.
    • Observations relative fecundity WILD, ADULTS: 141,000 eggs/kg at 14.6 cm fork length, 33,000 eggs/kg at 42.2 cm fork length: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2, 50-146 eggs/g at age 4-15: Loch Kinord and Loch Davan, Scotland 5, mean 105,271 at age 2+, mean 132,622 ate age 3+, mean 131,252 at age 4+: lake Varese, north-west Italy 11.

11.5 Brood care, breeding

Breeding type: lake spawner 
  • Observations: D1.



12  Senses

12.1 Vision

Visible spectrum: mainly green, red, but also violet (further research needed) 
  • LAB, ADULTS: projecting a stimulus onto the right or left side of a screen in front of the tank, ADULTS responded by putting their head into one of two response chambers and received reward if correct. After two consecutive correct responses, stimulus intensity decreased; after incorrect response, stimulus intensity increased. ADULTS were sensitive to wavelengths 400-750 with peaks at 530-560 nm (green) and 660-680 nm (red) and a smaller peak at 400 nm (violet) 40.
Importance of vision: predator avoidance (further research needed) 
  • Vision and predator avoidance:
    • LAB: three naive 0+ JUVENILES from wild-caught eggs in 30 x 30 x 20 cm 10 L aquaria with predator pike Esox lucius in transparent bag. During 15 min, decrease in food intake compared to control group (350-450 µg/IND/15 min each of large and small prey Daphnia magna versus 500-600 µg/IND/15 min). Sharper decrease (150-200 µg/IND/15 min each of large and small prey Daphnia magna versus 500 µg/IND/15 min) when additionally 150-200 mL water from aquarium with food-deprived pike dripped in indicating synergistic effect of visual and olfactory cues 12.
Tank colour and growth: better visibility via contrast of light food in dark tanks or vice versa 
  • LAB: LARVAE from wild-caught eggs in either black or grey 100 L flow-through tanks at 100-150 lux light intensity fed Artemia nauplii. After five weeks, higher weight (51.1 mg versus 23.8 mg) and higher length (19.1 mm versus 15.7 mm) in black than grey tanks. No difference in mortality (ca 75%). Results indicate visual foraging and better visibility via contrast of light nauplii in dark tanks 33.
  • LAB: JUVENILES in 0.1 m3 tanks lined with either black, grey, or white PVC sheets and light intensity of 1,117 or 222 lux fed dark food pellets. After three weeks, no effect of tank colour under 1,117 lux intensity; under 222 lux, higher feed intake (ca 1.0% versus 0.7% group average weight/IND/d) and higher specific growth rate (ca 0.7 versus 0.3) in white compared to black tanks, grey tanks in between. No difference in energy expenditure between light intensities indicates no stress. Results indicate visual foraging and better visibility via contrast of dark feed in lighter tanks 38.

12.2 Olfaction (and taste, if present)

Importance of olfaction: predator avoidance 
  • Olfaction and predator avoidance:
    • LAB: three naive 0+ JUVENILES from wild-caught eggs in 30 x 30 x 20 cm 10 L aquaria. During 15 min, 150-200 mL water from aquarium with food-deprived pike Esox lucius dripped in. No effect on food intake in JUVENILES. Decrease in food intake compared to control group (150-200 µg/IND/15 min each of large and small prey Daphnia magna versus 500 µg/IND/15 min) only when pike was additionally in the aquarium in a transparent bag. Results indicate that visual cues may be more important than olfactory cues in predator avoidance, maybe because in turbulent waters predator scent is dispersed in all directions and not reliable 12.

12.3 Hearing

Hearing type, hearing spectrum: hearing generalist, 0.1-0.3 kHz (further research needed) 
  • Hearing type:
    • LAB, JUVENILES-ADULTS: hearing GENERALIST 41 42.
  • Hearing spectrum:
    • LAB: JUVENILES-ADULTS in a plastic tub with diameter 33 cm and 13 cm depth lined with acoustically absorbent material. Audiogram under control laboratory conditions and playing back noise from still (40-60 dB), slow flowing (60-80 dB), and fast flowing rivers (80-100 dB). Greatest hearing sensitivity at 0.1-0.3 kHz with hearing threshold at 81.7-87.7 dB re 1 µPa. Combination with noise from different habitats did not influence hearing sensitivity but increased hearing threshold by 0-7.2 dB at two still waters and 5.2-12.5 dB at fast flowing river, slow flowing in between 41.
    • LAB: ADULTS in an aluminium tube ending with rubber membranes connected to piston and vibrator were exposed to particle movement by moving the pistons. ADULTS reacted to infrasound from 30 to 0.3 Hz ( D14). Blocking the lateral line system by exposing ADULTS to water containing Co2+ did not affect the hearing thresholds. Hearing infrasound probably involves the inner ear, not the lateral line system 7.
Noise and stress: sensitive to fluctuating noise (in amplitude and frequency) (further research needed) 
  • LAB: JUVENILES in 3 L bucket were exposed to boat noise (recorded in Danube river and lakes Mondsee and Traunsee, 153 dB), Gaussian noise (by noise generator, 156 dB), and no noise for 30 min. 99% increase in water cortisol under boat noise compared to no noise (ca 0.3 versus 0.2 ng/L water/g body weight), no increase under Gaussian noise. Results indicate higher influence of more fluctuating (in amplitude and frequency) compared to continuous sound 42.

12.4 Touch, mechanical sensing

No data found yet.

12.5 Lateral line

No data found yet.

12.6 Electrical sensing

No data found yet.

12.7 Nociception, pain sensing

No data found yet.

12.8 Other

No data found yet.


13  Communication

13.1 Visual

Colouration and habitat: increasing colouration with increasing water transparency (further research needed) 
  • WILD: ADULTS in Finnish lakes Harkkojärvi, Kinnasjärvi, Koppelojärvi, and Tuopanjärvi. In moderately humic lake Tuopanjärvi, more lateral and dorsal yellowness, more colourful and lighter tails, and more colourful and darker bellies in littoral than pelagic ADULTS. In humic lakes Kinnasjärvi and Koppelojärvi, more yellow and more red lateral and dorsal skin in littoral than pelagic ADULTS. In most humic lake Harkkojärvi, more red lateral and dorsal skin in littoral than pelagic ADULTS. Males with redder, yellower, and darker bellies than females only in most transparent lake. Results indicate colouration is adjusted to habitat – colour for intra-specific communication in littoral, dullness for predator avoidance in pelagic – and that difference is most pronounced in most transparent lake 20.

13.2 Chemical

No data found yet.

13.3 Acoustic

No data found yet.

13.4 Mechanical

No data found yet.

13.5 Electrical

No data found yet.

13.6 Other

No data found yet.


14  Social behaviour

14.1 Spatial organisation

Aggregation type: shoaling with increasing body size, as juveniles and adults schooling (further research needed) 
  • Observations shoal WILD, LARVAE-JUVENILES: in May, 3-6 m thick layer of bathypelagic LARVAE-JUVENILES of average 10.4 mm TOTAL LENGTH along 29 km of Slapy reservoir, Czech Republic, in 6.5-12.5 m depth. No shoaling. In June, 1-7 m thick layer of bathypelagic LARVAE-JUVENILES of average 31.8 mm TOTAL LENGTH along 52 km of Orik reservoir, Czech Republic, in 4.5-18.5 m depth 4.
  • Observations school WILD, JUVENILES-ADULTS: up to 40 of similar size and both sexes schooled during the day: lake Pounui, New Zealand (introduced) 2.
Stocking density in the wild: 0.1-8.1 ind/m, 30-89,111 ind/ha (further research needed) 
  • Observations WILD: layer of bathypelagic LARVAE-JUVENILES with 11,699-61,123 IND/ha or 0.4-2.6 IND/m: Slapy reservoir, Czech Republic, 2,197-89,111 IND/ha or 0.1-8.1 IND/m: Orlik reservoir, Czech Republic 4.
  • Observations WILD, FRY: 90 IND/100 m in 0-2 m and average 20.4 °C, 26 IND/100 m in 7-9 m and average 12.9 °C: Rimov reservoir, Czech Republic 14.
  • Observations WILD, JUVENILES: estimated at 50-150 IND/ha of age 0+, 300-3,500 IND/ha of age 1+, 30-1,000 IND/ha of age 2+: lake Abborrtjärn 3, Sweden 16.
  • WILD: stunted growth in forest lake Horkkajärvi with density 1,750 IND/ha of JUVENILES-ADULTS >8.5 cm compared to forest lake Nimetön (both in southern Finland) with 530 IND/ha 1. Authors suggested higher food competition due to higher density in Horkkajärvi as one of reasons for stunting.
Stocking density and stress: inverse relation below 10,000 ind/cage or 25-30 kg/m3 (further research needed) 
  • For stocking density and...
    ...cannibalism D4,
    ...aggression D15.

14.2 Social organisation

Social organisation type: linear hierarchy (when in small groups) (further research needed) 
  • Hierarchy and group size: in small groups, JUVENILES establish linear dominance hierarchies:
    • LAB: in groups of three 0+ JUVENILES in 10 L aquaria with grey plastic tube (3 cm diameter, 8 cm length, open on one side) at bottom, one became dominant; in eight of 10 aquaria, one to two became subordinate; in four of 10 aquaria, one to two became rival, meaning JUVENILES did not avoid aggressive encounters with dominant and entered dominant's territory 6.
  • Establishing hierarchy:
    • LAB: groups of three 0+ JUVENILES in 10 L aquaria with grey plastic tube (3 cm diameter, 8 cm length, open on one side) at bottom. Whereas JUVENILES usually entered tube together after 5-30 min, left after 1-2 h as dominant, subordinate, or rival ( Hierarchy and group size). Social position might still change after 6-8 h. From 10 h on, fixed 6.
Features of dominance: defend shelter, higher feed intake (further research needed) 
  • Features of dominance:
    • LAB: dominant JUVENILES defended shelter, had higher feeding intake than subordinate (15.0-17.1 Daphnia magna/IND/min versus 4.1-4.1 Daphnia magna/IND/min) 6.
Features of subordination: hardly move, avoid contact with the dominant (further research needed) 
  • Features of subordination:
    • LAB: subordinate JUVENILES avoided encounters, stayed in corner of aquarium or at surface 6.
  • Hierarchy and stress: no data found yet.

14.3 Exploitation

Cannibalism, predation: contribution to diet differs, most likely depending on habitat overlap and thus food competition 
  • WILD: contribution of 0+ JUVENILES to diet: in 1+ JUVENILES: 0-10%, in 3-5+ JUVENILES: 0% in some years to 100% in others. Most likely not due to decreased Zooplankton density but to (inshore) habitat overlap – and thus food competition – and increased energy demand: lake Abborrtjärn 3, Sweden 16.
  • WILD: given proportion of JUVENILES with gape larger than average body depth, cannibalism risk in five Swedish lakes ranged from 0.2% in lake Fisksjön to 73.6% in lake Ängersjön 17.
  • WILD: estimated daily cannibalistic attack rate: >20% for 1-6 cm JUVENILES with peak of 100% for 2.5 cm JUVENILES in lake Fisksjön, Sweden, >10% for 1.5-8 cm JUVENILES with peak of 30% for 3.5 cm JUVENILES in lake Ängersjön, Sweden 19.
  • WILD, JUVENILES-ADULTS: little cannibalism, only two cases observed: forest lakes Horkkajärvi and Nimetön, southern Finland 1.
  • FARM: 0+ JUVENILES stocked in natural ponds of 0.4-0.7 ha as eggs. At 25-40 days post hatch, depletion of Zooplankton. At 125 days post hatch, larger size bimodality in 0+ JUVENILES stocked with bream Abramis brama (up to 11 cm difference versus ca 4 cm) than 0+ JUVENILES without bream. Result indicates that after depletion of main food source, large 0+ JUVENILES turned to feeding on bream larvae and gained more energy which increased bimodality and enabled cannibalism 28.
  • LAB: eggs from same wild-caught strand in 100 L floating cages at stocking densities 1,000, 3,163, 10,000 eggs/cage. Partial cannibalism where the heads remained started at day 10 post hatching, complete cannibalism between days 14 and 18. After 22 days, bimodal weight distribution with FRY <11.2 mg deemed "prey", FRY >mean 51.2 mg deemed "cannibals". In prey, tendency of higher weight in high stocking density than low density (11.2 mg versus 7.3 mg), medium density in between (9.7 mg); in cannibals, tendency of higher weight in high and medium compared to low density (68.1-74.5 mg versus 51.2 mg). Tendency of higher survival in high density compared to low density (20.1% versus 14.8%), medium in between (18.9%), due mainly to tendency of lower cannibalism in medium and high compared to low density (28-34.4% versus 53.2%). Higher proportion of cannibals in final population in low than medium and high density (20.9% versus 4.2-9%). No difference in partial cannibalism with remaining heads (1.4-1.5%). Tendency of higher natural mortality in high and medium compared to low density (44.1-51.7% versus 30.7%) 10.
  • LAB: 10 day old LARVAE in 30 L tanks at 50 IND/L originating from one of two females and either an XY or XX male in groups of mixed-sex full siblings, all-female full siblings, mixed-sex half-siblings, or all-female half-siblings. After 77 days, no difference in growth (5.1-6.1 g). Higher survival in mixed-sex and all-female full siblings from one female than the other (59-61% versus 36-41%). Peaks of incomplete cannibalism, where the heads remain, in weeks 1-2 and 5-6. Higher frequency of incomplete cannibalism in mixed-sex or all-female full siblings from one female than the other (27.2-45.1% versus 6.6-20.2%). Results indicate genetic component in survival and cannibalism but authors refer to small sample size of four broodstock ADULTS 15.

14.4 Facilitation

No data found yet.

14.5 Aggression

Aggression and stocking density: inverse relation below 25-30 kg/m3 (further research needed) 
  • FARM, JUVENILES: densities of 25-30 kg/m3 decreased aggression personal communication Thomas Janssens 2017.
For aggression (or lack thereof)...
...and environmental enrichment D16,
...courtship D17.


14.6 Territoriality

No data found yet.


15  Cognitive abilities

15.1 Learning

Classical conditioning: may be used for measuring perception 
  • LAB: in a group of 5, ADULTS learned to associate a conditioned stimulus (a sound) – in place of the unconditioned stimulus of a mild electric shock to the tail – with a conditioned response (stress = decrease in heart rate). Allowed determination of hearing spectrum ( D18) 7.
Operant or instrumental conditioning: may be used for managing self-feeder, measuring perception 
  • Managing self-feeder:
    • LAB: ADULTS learned how to manage a self-feeder by pulling a piece of lead attached to a string of wool ( D6) 8.
  • For operant conditioning and visible spectrum D19.

15.2 Memory

No data found yet.

15.3 Problem solving, creativity, planning, intelligence

No data found yet.

15.4 Other

Predator recognition: influences feeding behaviour (further research needed) 
  • WILD/LAB: groups of four wild-caught JUVENILES in 95 x 41 x 44 cm 170 L aquaria with gravel and artificial vegetation, separated from ADULTS by a net. 0+ JUVENILES from lake Fiskjön, Sweden, with higher predation risk through cannibalism spent less time in the open than 1+ JUVENILES with lower risk. 0+ JUVENILES from lake Ängersjön, Sweden, with higher cannibalism risk tended to spend longer in first feeding bout than 1+ JUVENILES with lower risk. Probably because risk for predation by pike Esox lucius is higher for 1+ JUVENILES. JUVENILES across ages and lakes had lower latency to start feeding on the second than first observation day indicating that JUVENILES realised the low risk of the restrained predator 19.
  • For predator recognition and...
    ...importance of vision D20,
    ...importance of olfaction D21.



16  Personality, coping styles

No data found yet.


17  Emotion-like states

17.1 Joy

No data found yet.

17.2 Relaxation

No data found yet.

17.3 Sadness

No data found yet.

17.4 Fear

No data found yet.


18  Self-concept, self-recognition

No data found yet.


19  Reactions to husbandry

19.1 Stereotypical and vacuum activities

No data found yet.

19.2 Acute stress

Handling: air exposure is stressful (further research needed) 
  • Air exposure:
    • LAB: JUVENILES in 300 L tanks were caught, netted, and kept out of the water for 1 min. 30 min later, cortisol level had increased compared to control group (ca 120 versus 45 ng/mL) indicating stress. Decrease between observation time 30 min and 1 h, back to basal level at next observation time 4 h after handling. Tendency of increasing glucose levels with peak at 1 h after handling (55.9 versus ca 36 mg/dL) indicate increased energy expenditure. Increase in haematocrit with peak at 24 h after handling (21.2% versus 14.1%) indicates increase in oxygen transport efficiency to accommodate higher energy expenditure. Comparably high basal cortisol levels indicate lower adaptability to culture conditions 36.
    • LAB: 1+ JUVENILES of fourth captive generation of wild breeders in 100 L tanks that were drained and objected JUVENILES to 30 s air exposure. At 1 h after stressor, cortisol had increased compared to control group (57.3 versus 20 ng/mL) indicating stress. Back to control group levels at second observation time 6 h post stressor. At 1 and 6 h after stressor, glucose levels had increased compared to control group (1,150-1,243.7 versus 500-700 µg/mL). Back to control goup levels at third observation time 24 h post stressor. Decreased spleno-somatic index compared to control group (ca 0.06-0.08% versus 0.08-0.09%) at all observation times until 48 h post stressor indicates contracting spleen to provide blood with additional erythrocytes 39.
Live transport: stressful (further research needed) 
  • LAB: transporting JUVENILES for 4 h at 8 kg/m3 increased cortisol level compared to control group (145.7 versus ca 50 ng/mL) for at least two days afterwards indicating stress. Decrease between observation day 2 and 7 after transport. Glucose increased with peak at day 2 after transport (70.9 versus 50 mg/dL) indicating increased energy expenditure; back to basal level at next observation day 7. Increase of haematocrit with peak at day 2 after transport (23.9% versus 18.4%) indicates increase in oxygen transport efficiency to accommodate higher energy expenditure. Decrease in osmolality until observation day 21 (ca 0.3 versus 0.35 mOsm/kg) indicates disturbed gill osmotic balance. Comparably high basal cortisol levels indicate lower adaptability to culture conditions 36.
For acute stress and noise D22.


19.3 Chronic stress

Handling: no effect of domestication (further research needed) 
  • LAB: 1+ JUVENILES of first or fourth captive generation of wild breeders in 100 L tanks exposed to water emersion 3 times/week. On day 9, 48 h after the last stress event, lower cortisol levels in first generation JUVENILES compared to control group (4.7 versus 14.7 ng/mL); no difference in fourth generation (5.1-15.3 ng/mL). No differences on observation days 18 and 44 (6.0-24.9 ng/mL). No differences in glucose levels (385-727 µg/mL), spleno-somatic indices (0.05-0.06%), and serum lysozyme activity across generations and observation times. Increase in several proteins in serum indicate increased resistance to stressor. Results indicate no effect of domestication on physiological and immune responses to repeated handling 39.
Husbandry disturbance: stressful (further research needed) 
  • LAB: 0+ JUVENILES from wild-caught eggs in 100 dm3 tanks with either 22.7 °C or 16.6 °C water temperature. Tanks either undisturbed by wrapping in black plastic sheets, moderately disturbed by hand movements over the tank three times per day, or severely disturbed by hand movements three times per day and simulated cleaning with a brush once per day. After three rounds of three weeks each, lower feed intake (0.6-0.9 versus 0.8-1.1% group average weight/IND/day) and lower growth rate (0.3-0.5 versus 0.7-0.9) in severely compared to undisturbed tanks, independent of temperature; moderately disturbed tanks in between. Higher feed intake (0.8-1.1 versus 0.6-0.8% group average weight/IND/d) and higher growth rate (0.5-0.9 versus 0.3-0.7) under 22.7 than 16.6 °C. Decreasing feed intake (0.5-0.8 versus 0.9% group average weight/IND/d) and tendency of decreasing growth rate (0.4-0.6 versus 0.6-0.7) – lower at 16.6 compared to 22.7 °C – with increasing round indicating increased stress by long-term disturbance 37.
  • LAB: ADULTS in 80 L aquarium with pebbles and plants were held under 12 h light (07:00-19:00 h), 12 h dark cycle and exposed to stress by people walking by at irregular times during the day. Over 32 d, almost no trigger of the self-feeder during photophase. Most frequent triggering (average 8 triggers/h) in first hour of scotophase (19:00-20:00 h). Thereafter decrease in triggers from 3 triggers/h in 20:00-21:00 h until around 1 trigger/h in remaining scotophase 8.
For chronic stress and...
...environmental enrichment D16,
...nocturnal lighting D23,
...stocking density D24.


19.4 Stunning reactions

Stunning rules: fast, effective, safe 
  • Stunning rules: to minimise pain reactions and enhance welfare before slaughter:
    1. induce insensibility as fast as possible,
    2. prevent recovery from stunning,
    3. monitor effectiveness (observations, neurophysiological measurements) 45.
Stunning methods: percussive and electrical stunning most effective (further research needed) 
  • To minimise pain reactions, enhance welfare, and reduce the impact on the quality of the fish meat, these are across species the most efficient stunning methods 45 46:
    a) percussive stunning (if immediately followed by exsanguination),
    b) electrical stunning (if immediately followed by exsanguination),
    c) anaesthetics (clove oil derivants),
    d) spiking (if immediately followed by exsanguination),
    e) shooting,
    but only a) and b) are adaptable to industrial scale, whereas c) is still not admitted for food purposes in Europe.
    Further research needed for a specific protocol for P. fluviatilis.



Glossary


ADULTS = mature individuals
FARM = setting in farming environment or under conditions simulating farming environment in terms of size of facility or number of individuals
FRY = larvae from external feeding on
GENERALIST = Generalists detect a narrow bandwidth of sound frequencies (<50-500 Hz, 1,500 Hz max.). High hearing threshold = cannot detect quieter sounds. Typically no swim bladder or no attachment of the swim bladder to the inner ear. Live in loud environments (rivers) 43 44.
IND = individuals
JUVENILES = fully developed but immature individuals
LAB = setting in laboratory environment
LARVAE = hatching to mouth opening
MILLIARD = 1,000,000,000 26 27
TOTAL LENGTH = from snout to tip of caudal fin as compared to fork length (which measures from snout to fork of caudal fin) or standard length (from head to base of tail fin) or body length (from the base of the eye notch to the posterior end of the telson) 34
WILD = setting in the wild



Bibliography


1 Rask, Martti. 1983. Differences in growth of perch (Perca fluviatilis L.) in two small forest lakes. Hydrobiologia 101: 139–143. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00008666.
2 Jellyman, D. J. 1980. Age, growth, and reproduction of perch, Perca fluviatilis L., in Lake Pounui. New Zealand Journal of Marine and Freshwater Research 14: 391–400. https://doi.org/10.1080/00288330.1980.9515881.
3 Craig, John F. 1977. Seasonal changes in the day and night activity of adult perch, Perca fluviatilis L. Journal of Fish Biology 11: 161–166.
4 Čech, M., J. Kubečka, J. Frouzová, V. Draštík, M. Kratochvíl, J. Matěna, and J. Hejzlar. 2007. Distribution of the bathypelagic perch fry layer along the longitudinal profile of two large canyon-shaped reservoirs. Journal of Fish Biology 70: 141–154. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8649.2006.01282.x.
5 Treasurer, J. W. 1981. Some aspects of the reproductive biology of perch Perca fluviatilis L. Fecundity, maturation and spawning behaviour. Journal of Fish Biology 18: 729–740. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8649.1981.tb03814.x.
6 Mikheev, Victor N., Anna F. Pasternak, Gerhard Tischler, and Josef Wanzenböck. 2005. Contestable shelters provoke aggression among 0+ perch, Perca fluviatilis. Environmental Biology of Fishes 73: 227–231. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10641-005-0558-8.
7 Karlsen, Hanserik. 1992. The Inner Ear is Responsible for Detection of Infrasound in the Perch (Perca Fluviatilis). Journal of Experimental Biology 171: 163–172.
8 Ferter, Keno, and Victor Benno Meyer-Rochow. 2010. Turning Night into Day: Effects of Stress on the Self-Feeding Behaviour of the Eurasian Perch Perca fluviatilis. Zoological Studies 49: 176–181.
9 Reviewed distribution maps for European perch (Perca fluviatilis). 2016. Aquamaps.
10 Baras, Etienne, Patrick Kestemont, and Charles Mélard. 2003. Effect of stocking density on the dynamics of cannibalism in sibling larvae of Perca fluviatilis under controlled conditions. Aquaculture 219: 241–255. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0044-8486(02)00349-6.
11 Ceccuzzi, Pietro, Genciana Terova, Fabio Brambilla, Micaela Antonini, and Marco Saroglia. 2011. Growth, diet, and reproduction of Eurasian perch Perca fluviatilis L. in Lake Varese, northwestern Italy. Fisheries Science 77: 533–545. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12562-011-0353-8.
12 Mikheev, V. N., J. Wanzenböck, and A. F. Pasternak. 2006. Effects of predator-induced visual and olfactory cues on 0+ perch (Perca fluviatilis L.) foraging behaviour. Ecology of Freshwater Fish 15: 111–117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0633.2006.00140.x.
13 Čech, M., J. Peterka, M. Říha, L. Vejřík, T. Jůza, M. Kratochvíl, V. Draštík, M. Muška, P. Znachor, and J. Kubečka. 2012. Extremely shallow spawning of perch (Perca fluviatilis L.): the roles of sheltered bays, dense semi-terrestrial vegetation and low visibility in deeper water. Knowledge and Management of Aquatic Ecosystems 406: 12. https://doi.org/10.1051/kmae/2012026.
14 Petrtýl, Miloslav, Lukáš Kalous, Jaroslava Frouzová, and Martin Čech. 2015. Effects of habitat type on short- and long-term growth parameters of the European perch (Perca fluviatilis L.): Size distribution and RNA/DNA ratio of European perch fry. International Review of Hydrobiology 100: 13–20. https://doi.org/10.1002/iroh.201301675.
15 Król, Jaroslaw, Nicolas Dauchot, Syaghalirwa N M Mandiki, Pierre van Cutsem, and Patrick Kestemont. 2015. Cannibalism in cultured eurasian perch, Perca fluviatilis (Actinopterygii: Perciformes: Percidae)-Implication of maternal influence, kinship, and sex ratio of progenies. Acta Ichthyologica et Piscatoria 45: 65–73. https://doi.org/10.3750/AIP2015.45.1.07.
16 Persson, Lennart, Pär Byström, and Eva Wahlström. 2000. Cannibalism and Competition in Eurasian Perch: Population Dynamics of an Ontogenetic Omnivore. Ecology 81: 1058–1071. https://doi.org/10.1890/0012-9658(2000)081[1058:CACIEP]2.0.CO;2.
17 Magnhagen, C., and E. Heibo. 2004. Growth in length and in body depth in young-of-the-year perch with different predation risk. Journal of Fish Biology 64: 612–624. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8649.2004.00325.x.
18 Heibo, Erik, Carin Magnhagen, and Leif Asbjørn Vøllestad. 2005. Latitudinal variation in life-history traits in Eurasian perch. Ecology 86: 3377–3386. https://doi.org/10.1890/04-1620.
19 Magnhagen, Carin, and Jost Borcherding. 2008. Risk-taking behaviour in foraging perch: does predation pressure influence age-specific boldness? Animal Behaviour 75: 509–517. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.06.007.
20 Kekäläinen, Jukka, Hannu Huuskonen, Vesa Kiviniemi, and Jouni Taskinen. 2010. Visual conditions and habitat shape the coloration of the Eurasian perch (Perca fluviatilis L.): a trade-off between camouflage and communication? Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 99: 47–59. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8312.2009.01339.x.
21 Froese, R., and D. Pauly. 2014. FishBase. World Wide Web electronic publication. www.fishbase.org.
22 FAO. 2014. The State of World Fisheries and Aquaculture 2014. Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations.
23 Watson, R., Jackie Alder, and Daniel Pauly. 2006. Fisheries for forage fish, 1950 to the present. In On the Multiple Uses of Forage Fish: from Ecosystems to Markets, ed. Jackie Alder and Daniel Pauly, 14:1–20. Fisheries Centre Research Reports 3. Vancouver, Canada: Fisheries Centre, University of British Columbia.
24 Mood, A. 2012. Average annual fish capture for species mostly used for fishmeal (2005-2009). fishcount.org.uk.
25 Mood, A., and P. Brooke. 2012. Estimating the Number of Farmed Fish Killed in Global Aquaculture Each Year.
26 Kopf, Von Kristin. 2012. Milliarden vs. Billionen: Große Zahlen. Sprachlog.
27 Weisstein, Eric W. 2018. Milliard. Text. MathWorld - a Wolfram Web resource. http://mathworld.wolfram.com/Milliard.html. Accessed February 2.
28 Heermann, Lisa, Werner Scharf, Gerard van der Velde, and Jost Borcherding. 2014. Does the use of alternative food resources induce cannibalism in a size-structured fish population? Ecology of Freshwater Fish 23: 129–140. https://doi.org/10.1111/eff.12060.
29 Brüning, Anika, Franz Hölker, Steffen Franke, Torsten Preuer, and Werner Kloas. 2015. Spotlight on fish: Light pollution affects circadian rhythms of European perch but does not cause stress. Science of The Total Environment 511: 516–522. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2014.12.094.
30 Le Cren, E. D. 1958. Observations on the Growth of Perch (Perca fluviatilis L.) Over Twenty-Two Years with Special Reference to the Effects of Temperature and Changes in Population Density. Journal of Animal Ecology 27: 287–334. https://doi.org/10.2307/2242.
31 Hunt, Darcie Elizabeth. 2015. The effect of visual capacity and swimming ability of fish on the performance of light-based bycatch reduction devices in prawn trawls. Doctoral dissertation, University of Tasmania.
32 Voskoboinikova, O. S., and I. G. Grechanov. 2002. Development of the Skeleton during the Ontogenesis of the River Perch Perca fluviatilis. Journal of Ichthyology 42: 322–333.
33 Jentoft, Sissel, Sigurd ØXnevad, Are H. Aastveit, and ØIvind Andersen. 2006. Effects of Tank Wall Color and Up-welling Water Flow on Growth and Survival of Eurasian Perch Larvae (Perca fluviatilis). Journal of the World Aquaculture Society 37: 313–317. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-7345.2006.00042.x.
34 Pawson, M.G., and G.D. Pickett. 1996. The Annual Pattern of Condition and Maturity in Bass, Dicentrarchus Labrax, in Waters Around England and Wales. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 76: 107. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025315400029040.
35 Konstantinov, K. G. 1957. Comparative analysis of morphology and biology of perch, pikeperch, Volga pikeperch of different stages of development. Rep. Inst. Evol. Morph. Anim. 16: 181–236.
36 Acerete, L, J. C Balasch, E Espinosa, A Josa, and L Tort. 2004. Physiological responses in Eurasian perch (Perca fluviatilis, L.) subjected to stress by transport and handling. Aquaculture 237: 167–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaculture.2004.03.018.
37 Strand, Å., C. Magnhagen, and A. Alanärä. 2007. Effects of repeated disturbances on feed intake, growth rates and energy expenditures of juvenile perch, Perca fluviatilis. Aquaculture 265: 163–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaculture.2007.01.030.
38 Strand, Å., A. Alanärä, F. Staffan, and C. Magnhagen. 2007. Effects of tank colour and light intensity on feed intake, growth rate and energy expenditure of juvenile Eurasian perch, Perca fluviatilis L. Aquaculture 272: 312–318. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaculture.2007.08.052.
39 Douxfils, J., S. Lambert, C. Mathieu, S. Milla, S. N. M. Mandiki, E. Henrotte, N. Wang, et al. 2014. Influence of domestication process on immune response to repeated emersion stressors in Eurasian perch (Perca fluviatilis, L.). Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology 173: 52–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpa.2014.03.012.
40 Cameron, N. E. 1982. The photopic spectral sensitivity of a dichromatic teleost fish (perca fluviatilis). Vision Research 22: 1341–1348. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(82)90223-1.
41 Amoser, Sonja, and Friedrich Ladich. 2005. Are hearing sensitivities of freshwater fish adapted to the ambient noise in their habitats? Journal of Experimental Biology 208: 3533–3542. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.01809.
42 Wysocki, Lidia Eva, John P. Dittami, and Friedrich Ladich. 2006. Ship noise and cortisol secretion in European freshwater fishes. Biological Conservation 128: 501–508. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2005.10.020.
43 Brown, Culum. 2015. Fish intelligence, sentience and ethics. Animal Cognition 18: 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0761-0.
44 Amundsen, Lasse, and Martin Landro. 2011. Marine seismic sources part VIII: Fish hear a great deal. Recent Advances in Technology 8: 1–5.
45 Robb, D H F, and S C Kestin. 2002. Methods Used to Kill Fish: Field Observations and Literature Reviewed. Animal Welfare 11: 269–282.
46 Stamer, Andreas. 2009. Betäubungs- und Schlachtmethoden für Speisefische. Report.


contents
show all details
«