Information
Author: Pablo Arechavala-Lopez
Version: B | 1.2Published: 2024-12-31
- minor editorial changes plus new side note "Commercial relevance"
WelfareScore | farm
The score card gives our welfare assessments for aquatic species in 10 criteria.
For each criterion, we score the probability to experience good welfare under minimal farming conditions ("Likelihood") and under high-standard farming conditions ("Potential") representing the worst and best case scenario. The third dimension scores how certain we are of our assessments based on the number and quality of sources we found ("Certainty").
The WelfareScore sums just the "High" scores in each dimension. Although good welfare ("High") seems not possible in some criteria, there could be at least a potential improvement from low to medium welfare (indicated by ➚ and the number of criteria).
- Li = Likelihood that the individuals of the species experience good welfare under minimal farming conditions
- Po = Potential of the individuals of the species to experience good welfare under high-standard farming conditions
➚ = potential improvements not reaching "High" - Ce = Certainty of our findings in Likelihood and Potential
WelfareScore = Sum of criteria scoring "High" (max. 10 per dimension)
General remarks
Octopus vulgaris has recently aroused much interest in aquaculture, considered suitable for large-scale production given its commercial value, its fecundity, rapid growth, high protein content, and high feed efficiency rate. The main problem, however, is the high mortality rate observed during paralarval rearing, making successful JUVENILES settlement still very difficult to achieve. Unfortunately, despite the high knowledge on the biology and ethology of this species, there are many other aspects to be solved from a welfare perspective. For instance, the current farming systems result in high stress in O. vulgaris due to spatial constraint, high densities, and sociability, which consequently increase aggression (cannibalism and autophagy) at different life stages. In addition, octopus skin is particularly sensitive and can be easily damaged during handling, transportation or stressful confinement conditions. A humane slaughtering protocol is not yet established, since the nature and degree of any suffering during current practices are unknown. O. vulgaris appears capable of experiencing pain and suffering, exhibits cognitive complexity and sophisticated behavioural patterns which can be interpreted and serve as indicator of the welfare status.
1 Home range
Many species traverse in a limited horizontal space (even if just for a certain period of time per year); the home range may be described as a species' understanding of its environment (i.e., its cognitive map) for the most important resources it needs access to.
What is the probability of providing the species' whole home range in captivity?
It is low for minimal and high-standard farming conditions. Our conclusion is based on a high amount of evidence.


2 Depth range
Given the availability of resources (food, shelter) or the need to avoid predators, species spend their time within a certain depth range.
What is the probability of providing the species' whole depth range in captivity?
It is low for minimal farming conditions. It is medium for high-standard farming conditions. Our conclusion is based on a high amount of evidence.


3 Migration
Some species undergo seasonal changes of environments for different purposes (feeding, spawning, etc.), and to move there, they migrate for more or less extensive distances.
What is the probability of providing farming conditions that are compatible with the migrating or habitat-changing behaviour of the species?
It is low for minimal farming conditions. It is medium for high-standard farming conditions. Our conclusion is based on a medium amount of evidence.


4 Reproduction
A species reproduces at a certain age, season, and sex ratio and possibly involving courtship rituals.
What is the probability of the species reproducing naturally in captivity without manipulation of these circumstances?
It is low for minimal farming conditions. It is medium for high-standard farming conditions. Our conclusion is based on a medium amount of evidence.


5 Aggregation
Species differ in the way they co-exist with conspecifics or other species from being solitary to aggregating unstructured, casually roaming in shoals or closely coordinating in schools of varying densities.
What is the probability of providing farming conditions that are compatible with the aggregation behaviour of the species?
It is low for minimal and high-standard farming conditions. Our conclusion is based on a high amount of evidence.


6 Aggression
There is a range of adverse reactions in species, spanning from being relatively indifferent towards others to defending valuable resources (e.g., food, territory, mates) to actively attacking opponents.
What is the probability of the species being non-aggressive and non-territorial in captivity?
It is low for minimal and high-standard farming conditions. Our conclusion is based on a medium amount of evidence.


7 Substrate
Depending on where in the water column the species lives, it differs in interacting with or relying on various substrates for feeding or covering purposes (e.g., plants, rocks and stones, sand and mud, turbidity).
What is the probability of providing the species' substrate and shelter needs in captivity?
It is low for minimal farming conditions. It is high for high-standard farming conditions. Our conclusion is based on a medium amount of evidence.


8 Stress
Farming involves subjecting the species to diverse procedures (e.g., handling, air exposure, short-term confinement, short-term crowding, transport), sudden parameter changes or repeated disturbances (e.g., husbandry, size-grading).
What is the probability of the species not being stressed?
It is low for minimal farming conditions. It is medium for high-standard farming conditions. Our conclusion is based on a medium amount of evidence.


9 Malformations
Deformities that – in contrast to diseases – are commonly irreversible may indicate sub-optimal rearing conditions (e.g., mechanical stress during hatching and rearing, environmental factors unless mentioned in crit. 3, aquatic pollutants, nutritional deficiencies) or a general incompatibility of the species with being farmed.
What is the probability of the species being malformed rarely?
There are no findings for minimal and high-standard farming conditions.


10 Slaughter
The cornerstone for a humane treatment is that slaughter a) immediately follows stunning (i.e., while the individual is unconscious), b) happens according to a clear and reproducible set of instructions verified under farming conditions, and c) avoids pain, suffering, and distress.
What is the probability of the species being slaughtered according to a humane slaughter protocol?
It is unclear for minimal farming conditions. It is low for high-standard farming conditions. Our conclusion is based on a medium amount of evidence.


Side note: Domestication
Teletchea and Fontaine introduced 5 domestication levels illustrating how far species are from having their life cycle closed in captivity without wild input, how long they have been reared in captivity, and whether breeding programmes are in place.
What is the species’ domestication level?
DOMESTICATION LEVEL 3 30, level 5 being fully domesticated.
Side note: Forage fish in the feed
450-1,000 milliard wild-caught fishes end up being processed into fish meal and fish oil each year which contributes to overfishing and represents enormous suffering. There is a broad range of feeding types within species reared in captivity.
To what degree may fish meal and fish oil based on forage fish be replaced by non-forage fishery components (e.g., poultry blood meal) or sustainable sources (e.g., soybean cake)?
All age classes: WILD: carnivorous 41 42 43 44 6 45. FARM: fish oil may be completely* omitted 46. Fish meal may be mostly* replaced by non-forage fishery components 47. In Octopus maya, fish meal may be completely* replaced by non-forage fishery components 48. Further research to determine whether this applies to O. vulgaris as well.
*partly = <51% – mostly = 51-99% – completely = 100%
Side note: Commercial relevance
How much is this species farmed annually?
No data found yet.Glossary
DOMESTICATION LEVEL 3 = entire life cycle closed in captivity with wild inputs 40
FARM = setting in farming environment or under conditions simulating farming environment in terms of size of facility or number of individuals
IND = individuals
JUVENILES = fully developed but immature individuals
LAB = setting in laboratory environment
LARVAE = hatching to mouth opening
PELAGIC = living independent of bottom and shore of a body of water
PHOTOPERIOD = duration of daylight
PLANKTONIC = horizontal movement limited to hydrodynamic displacement
SPAWNERS = adults during the spawning season; in farms: adults that are kept as broodstock
WILD = setting in the wild
Bibliography
2 Arechavala-Lopez, P., M. Minguito-Frutos, G. Follana-Berná, and M. Palmer. 2019. Common octopus settled in human-altered Mediterranean coastal waters: from individual home range to population dynamics. ICES Journal of Marine Science 76: 585–597. https://doi.org/10.1093/icesjms/fsy014.
3 Mereu, Marco, Blondine Agus, Rita Cannas, Alessandro Cau, Elisabetta Coluccia, and Danila Cuccu. 2015. Mark–recapture investigation on Octopus vulgaris specimens in an area of the central western Mediterranean Sea. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 95: 131–138. https://doi.org/10.1017/S002531541400112X.
4 Mereu, Marco, Blondine Agus, Piero Addis, Serenella Cabiddu, Alessandro Cau, Maria Cristina Follesa, and Danila Cuccu. 2015. Movement estimation of Octopus vulgaris Cuvier, 1797 from mark recapture experiment. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 470: 64–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2015.05.007.
5 Otero, Jaime, Ángel F. González, M. Pilar Sieiro, and Ángel Guerra. 2007. Reproductive cycle and energy allocation of Octopus vulgaris in Galician waters, NE Atlantic. Fisheries Research 85: 122–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fishres.2007.01.007.
6 Roura, Álvaro, Ángel F. González, Kevin Redd, and Ángel Guerra. 2012. Molecular prey identification in wild Octopus vulgaris paralarvae. Marine Biology 159: 1335–1345. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-012-1914-9.
7 Guerra, Ángel. 1981. Spatial distribution pattern of Octopus vulgaris. Journal of Zoology 195: 133–146.
8 Katsanevakis, Stelios, and George Verriopoulos. 2004. Abundance of Octopus vulgaris on soft sediment. Scientia Marina 68: 553–560. https://doi.org/10.3989/scimar.2004.68n4553.
9 Pierce, G. J., L. Allcock, I. Bruno, P. Bustamante, A. González, A. Guerra, P. Jereb, et al. 2010. Cephalopod biology and fisheries in Europe. Edited by G. J. Pierce, L. Allcock, I. Bruno, P. Bustamante, A. González, A. Guerra, P. Jereb, et al. Vol. 303. ICES Cooperative Research Report. Copenhagen, Denmark: ICES.
10 Guerra, Ángel, Jorge Hernández-Urcera, Manuel E. Garci, Marta Sestelo, Marcos Regueira, Ángel F. González, Miguel Cabanellas-Reboredo, Matías Calvo-Manazza, and Beatriz Morales-Nin. 2014. Dwellers in dens on sandy bottoms: Ecological and behavioural traits of Octopus vulgaris. Scientia Marina 78: 405–414. https://doi.org/10.3989/scimar.04071.28F.
11 Iglesias, J., F. J. Sánchez, J. G. F. Bersano, J. F. Carrasco, J. Dhont, L. Fuentes, F. Linares, et al. 2007. Rearing of Octopus vulgaris paralarvae: Present status, bottlenecks and trends. Aquaculture 266: 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaculture.2007.02.019.
12 Fiorito, Graziano, Andrea Affuso, Jennifer Basil, Alison Cole, Paolo de Girolamo, Livia D’Angelo, Ludovic Dickel, et al. 2015. Guidelines for the Care and Welfare of Cephalopods in Research –A consensus based on an initiative by CephRes, FELASA and the Boyd Group. Laboratory Animals 49: 1–90. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023677215580006.
13 Fuentes, Lidia, and José Iglesias-Estévez. 2010. Release experiments with Octopus vulgaris Cuvier, 1797 in Galicia, NW Spain. First results on recapture rate, distribution and growth. Vie et Milieu - Life and Environment 60: 65–71.
14 Aguado Giménez, F., and B. García García. 2002. Growth and food intake models in Octopus vulgaris Cuvier (1797): influence of body weight, temperature, sex and diet. Aquaculture International 10: 361–377. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023335024053.
15 Miliou, Helen, Myrsini Fintikaki, Triantaphyllos Kountouris, and George Verriopoulos. 2005. Combined effects of temperature and body weight on growth and protein utilization of the common octopus, Octopus vulgaris. Aquaculture 249: 245–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaculture.2005.03.038.
16 Delgado, Marina, Joan Ignasi Gairín, Ricard Carbó, and Cristóbal Aguilera. 2010. Growth of Octopus vulgaris (Cuvier, 1797) in tanks in the Ebro Delta (NE Spain): effects of temperature, salinity and culture density. Scientia Marina 75: 53–59. https://doi.org/10.3989/scimar.2011.75n1053.
17 Cuccu, Danila, Marco Mereu, Alessandro Cau, Paola Pesci, and Angelo Cau. 2013. Reproductive development versus estimated age and size in a wild Mediterranean population of Octopus vulgaris (Cephalopoda: Octopodidae). Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 93: 843–849. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025315412000203.
18 Katsanevakis, Stelios, and George Verriopoulos. 2006. Seasonal population dynamics of Octopus vulgaris in the eastern Mediterranean. ICES Journal of Marine Science 63: 151–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.icesjms.2005.07.004.
19 Caverivière, Alain, François Domain, and Anis Diallo. 1999. Observations on the influence of temperature on the length of embryonic development in Octopus vulgaris (Senegal). Aquatic Living Resources 12: 151–154. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0990-7440(99)80024-2.
20 Wolf, Tania De, Simone Lenzi, and Francesco Lenzi. 2011. Paralarval rearing of Octopus vulgaris (Cuvier) in Tuscany, Italy. Aquaculture Research 42: 1406–1414. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2109.2010.02756.x.
21 Mather, Jennifer A. 1994. ‘Home’ choice and modification by juvenile Octopus vulgaris (Mollusca: Cephalopoda): specialized intelligence and tool use? Journal of Zoology 233: 359–368. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.1994.tb05270.x.
22 Rodríguez, Carmen, José F. Carrasco, Juan C. Arronte, and Montse Rodríguez. 2006. Common octopus (Octopus vulgaris Cuvier, 1797) juvenile ongrowing in floating cages. Aquaculture 254: 293–300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaculture.2005.10.053.
23 Domingues, Pedro, Sandra Garcia, and Diego Garrido. 2010. Effects of three culture densities on growth and survival of Octopus vulgaris (Cuvier, 1797). Aquaculture International 18: 165–174. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10499-008-9233-3.
24 Chapela, Albert, Ángel F. González, Earl G. Dawe, Francisco J. Rocha, and Ángel Guerra. 2006. Growth of common octopus (Octopus vulgaris) in cages suspended from rafts. Scientia Marina 70: 121–129. https://doi.org/10.3989/scimar.2006.70n1121.
25 Roura, Álvaro, A. Castro Bugallo, M. Martínez Pérez, and A. F. González. 2019. Te quedaste con hambre? Canibalismo en la etapa larvaria del pulpo común. Libro de Actas del XVII Congreso Nacional de Acuicultura. Cartagena, Spain.
26 Altman, J. S., J. N. Lythgoe, and J. D. Woods. 1966. The behaviour of octopuis vulgaris Lam. in its natural habitat : a pilot study. Underwater Association Report. Carshalton, England: T.G.W. Industrial & Research Promotions.
27 Kayes, R. J. 1973. The daily activity pattern of Octopus vulgaris in a natural habitat. Marine Behaviour and Physiology 2: 337–343. https://doi.org/10.1080/10236247309386935.
28 Mather, Jennifer A., and Ron K. O’Dor. 1991. Foraging Strategies and Predation Risk Shape the Natural History of Juvenile Octopus Vulgaris. Bulletin of Marine Science 49: 256–269.
29 Hernández-Urcera, Jorge, Manuel E. Garci, Álvaro Roura, Ángel F. González, Miguel Cabanellas-Reboredo, Beatriz Morales-Nin, and Ángel Guerra. 2014. Cannibalistic behavior of octopus (Octopus vulgaris) in the wild. Journal of Comparative Psychology 128: 427–430. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036883.
30 Arechavala-Lopez, Pablo. 2019. Personal communication.
31 Mereu, Marco, Alessandro Cau, Blondine Agus, Rita Cannas, Maria Cristina Follesa, Paola Pesci, and Danila Cuccu. 2018. Artificial dens as a management tool for Octopus vulgaris: evidence from a Collaborative Fisheries Research project (central western Mediterranean Sea). Ocean & Coastal Management 165: 428–433. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ocecoaman.2018.09.006.
32 Iglesias, J., J.J. Otero, C. Moxica, L. Fuentes, and F.J. Sánchez. 2004. The Completed Life Cycle of the Octopus (Octopus vulgaris, Cuvier) under Culture Conditions: Paralarval Rearing using Artemia and Zoeae, and First Data on Juvenile Growth up to 8 Months of Age. Aquaculture International 12: 481–487. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:AQUI.0000042142.88449.bc.
33 García, Benjamín García, Jesús Cerezo Valverde, Felipe Aguado‐Giménez, José García García, and María D. Hernández. 2009. Growth and mortality of common octopus Octopus vulgaris reared at different stocking densities in Mediterranean offshore cages. Aquaculture Research 40: 1202–1212. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2109.2009.02222.x.
34 Estefanell, J., J. Roo, R. Guirao, M. Izquierdo, and J. Socorro. 2012. Benthic cages versus floating cages in Octopus vulgaris: Biological performance and biochemical composition feeding on Boops boops discarded from fish farms. Aquacultural Engineering 49: 46–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaeng.2012.02.001.
35 Estefanell, J., J. Roo, H. Fernández‐Palacios, M. Izquierdo, J. Socorro, and R. Guirao. 2012. Comparison Between Individual and Group Rearing Systems in Octopus vulgaris (Cuvier, 1797). Journal of the World Aquaculture Society 43: 63–72. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-7345.2011.00540.x.
36 Kwon, Inyeong, and Taeho Kim. 2015. Growth and mortality of the juvenile common octopus (Octopus vulgaris) in pipe-and tire-type shelters placed in flow-through seawater tanks. J Kor Soc Fish Technol 51: 1–8.
37 Guerra, Ángel, Jorge Hernández-Urcera, Manuel E. Garci, Marta Sestelo, Marcos Regueira, Ángel F. González, Miguel Cabanellas-Reboredo, Matías Calvo-Manazza, and Beatriz Morales-Nin. 2015. Spawning habitat selection by Octopus vulgaris: New insights for a more effective management of this resource. Fisheries Research 167: 313–322. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fishres.2015.03.011.
38 Boyle, P. R. 1991. The UFAW handbook on the care and management of cephalopods in the laboratory. Potters Bar, UK: Universities Federation for Animal Welfare.
39 Andrews, Paul L. R., Anne-Sophie Darmaillacq, Ngaire Dennison, Ian G. Gleadall, Penny Hawkins, John B. Messenger, Daniel Osorio, Valerie J. Smith, and Jane A. Smith. 2013. The identification and management of pain, suffering and distress in cephalopods, including anaesthesia, analgesia and humane killing. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 447. Cephalopod Biology a Special Issue Compiled under the Auspices of No-Profit Research Organization CephRes: 46–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2013.02.010.
40 Teletchea, Fabrice, and Pascal Fontaine. 2012. Levels of domestication in fish: implications for the sustainable future of aquaculture. Fish and Fisheries 15: 181–195. https://doi.org/10.1111/faf.12006.
41 Quetglas, Antoni, Francesc Alemany, Aina Carbonell, Paolo Merella, and Pilar Sánchez. 1998. Biology and fishery of Octopus vulgaris Cuvier, 1797, caught by trawlers in Mallorca (Balearic Sea, Western Mediterranean). Fisheries Research 36: 237–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-7836(98)00093-9.
42 Rosa, Rui, António Manuel Marques, María Leonor Nunes, Narcisa Bandarra, and Carlos Sousa Reis. 2004. Spatial-temporal changes in dimethyl acetal (octadecanal) levels of Octopus vulgaris (Mollusca, Cephalopoda): relation to feeding ecology. Scientia Marina 68: 227–236. https://doi.org/10.3989/scimar.2004.68n2227.
43 Villanueva, R., J. M. Escudero, R. Deulofeu, A. Bozzano, and C. Casoliva. 2009. Vitamin A and E content in early stages of cephalopods and their dietary effects in Octopus vulgaris paralarvae. Aquaculture 286: 277–282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaculture.2008.09.032.
44 Moreno, Ana, Antonina Dos Santos, Uwe Piatkowski, A. Miguel P. Santos, and Henrique Cabral. 2009. Distribution of cephalopod paralarvae in relation to the regional oceanography of the western Iberia. Journal of Plankton Research 31: 73–91. https://doi.org/10.1093/plankt/fbn103.
45 Olmos-Pérez, Lorena, Álvaro Roura, Graham J. Pierce, Stéphane Boyer, and Ángel F. González. 2017. Diet Composition and Variability of Wild Octopus vulgaris and Alloteuthis media (Cephalopoda) Paralarvae: a Metagenomic Approach. Frontiers in Physiology 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2017.00321.
46 Morillo‐Velarde, Piedad S., Jesús Cerezo Valverde, and Benjamín García‐García. 2015. Utilization of diets with different fish oil content in common octopus (Octopus vulgaris Cuvier, 1797) and resulting changes in its biochemical composition. Aquaculture Research 46: 2871–2884. https://doi.org/10.1111/are.12439.
47 Morillo-Velarde, P. S., J. Cerezo Valverde, M. D. Hernández, F. Aguado-Giménez, and B. García García. 2012. Growth and digestibility of formulated diets based on dry and freeze-dried ingredients in the common octopus (Octopus vulgaris). Aquaculture 368–369: 139–144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaculture.2012.09.028.
48 Méndez Aguilar, Francisco Daniel, Miguel Ángel Olvera Novoa, Sergio Rodríguez Morales, and Carlos Rosas Vázquez. 2014. Nutritive value of four by-product meals as potential protein sources in diets for Octopus maya. Hidrobiológica 24: 69–77.
Lorem ipsum
Something along the lines of: we were aware of the importance of some topics so that we wanted to include them and collect data but not score them. For WelfareChecks | farm, these topics are "domestication level", "feed replacement", and "commercial relevance". The domestication and commercial relevance aspects allow us to analyse the questions whether increasing rate of domestication or relevance in farming worldwide goes hand in hand with better welfare; the feed replacement rather goes in the direction of added suffering for all those species which end up as feed. For a carnivorous species, to gain 1 kg of meat, you do not just kill this one individual but you have to take into account the meat that it was fed during its life in the form of fish meal and fish oil. In other words, carnivorous species (and to a degree also omnivorous ones) have a larger "fish in:fish out" ratio.
Lorem ipsum
Probably, we updated the profile. Check the version number in the head of the page. For more information on the version, see the FAQ about this. Why do we update profiles? Not just do we want to include new research that has come out, but we are continuously developing the database itself. For example, we changed the structure of entries in criteria or we added explanations for scores in the WelfareCheck | farm. And we are always refining our scoring rules.
The centre of the Overview is an array of criteria covering basic features and behaviours of the species. Each of this information comes from our literature search on the species. If we researched a full Dossier on the species, probably all criteria in the Overview will be covered and thus filled. This was our way to go when we first set up the database.
Because Dossiers are time consuming to research, we switched to focusing on WelfareChecks. These are much shorter profiles covering just 10 criteria we deemed important when it comes to behaviour and welfare in aquaculture (and lately fisheries, too). Also, WelfareChecks contain the assessment of the welfare potential of a species which has become the main feature of the fair-fish database over time. Because WelfareChecks do not cover as many criteria as a Dossier, we don't have the information to fill all blanks in the Overview, as this information is "not investigated by us yet".
Our long-term goal is to go back to researching Dossiers for all species covered in the fair-fish database once we set up WelfareChecks for each of them. If you would like to support us financially with this, please get in touch at ffdb@fair-fish.net
See the question "What does "not investigated by us yet" mean?". In short, if we have not had a look in the literature - or in other words, if we have not investigated a criterion - we cannot know the data. If we have already checked the literature on a criterion and could not find anything, it is "no data found yet". You spotted a "no data found yet" where you know data exists? Get in touch with us at ffdb@fair-fish.net!
Once you have clicked on "show details", the entry for a criterion will unfold and display the summarised information we collected from the scientific literature – complete with the reference(s).
As reference style we chose "Springer Humanities (numeric, brackets)" which presents itself in the database as a number in a grey box. Mouse over the box to see the reference; click on it to jump to the bibliography at the bottom of the page. But what does "[x]-[y]" refer to?
This is the way we mark secondary citations. In this case, we read reference "y", but not reference "x", and cite "x" as mentioned in "y". We try to avoid citing secondary references as best as possible and instead read the original source ourselves. Sometimes we have to resort to citing secondarily, though, when the original source is: a) very old or not (digitally) available for other reasons, b) in a language no one in the team understands. Seldomly, it also happens that we are running out of time on a profile and cannot afford to read the original. As mentioned, though, we try to avoid it, as citing mistakes may always happen (and we don't want to copy the mistake) and as misunderstandings may occur by interpreting the secondarily cited information incorrectly.
If you spot a secondary reference and would like to send us the original work, please contact us at ffdb@fair-fish.net
In general, we aim at giving a good representation of the literature published on the respective species and read as much as we can. We do have a time budget on each profile, though. This is around 80-100 hours for a WelfareCheck and around 300 hours for a Dossier. It might thus be that we simply did not come around to reading the paper.
It is also possible, though, that we did have to make a decision between several papers on the same topic. If there are too many papers on one issue than we manage to read in time, we have to select a sample. On certain topics that currently attract a lot of attention, it might be beneficial to opt for the more recent papers; on other topics, especially in basic research on behaviour in the wild, the older papers might be the go-to source.
And speaking of time: the paper you are missing from the profile might have come out after the profile was published. For the publication date, please check the head of the profile at "cite this profile". We currently update profiles every 6-7 years.
If your paper slipped through the cracks and you would like us to consider it, please get in touch at ffdb@fair-fish.net
This number, for example "C | 2.1 (2022-11-02)", contains 4 parts:
- "C" marks the appearance – the design level – of the profile part. In WelfareChecks | farm, appearance "C" is our most recent one with consistent age class and label (WILD, FARM, LAB) structure across all criteria.
- "2." marks the number of major releases within this appearance. Here, it is major release 2. Major releases include e.g. changes of the WelfareScore. Even if we just add one paper – if it changes the score for one or several criteria, we will mark this as a major update for the profile. With a change to a new appearance, the major release will be re-set to 1.
- ".1" marks the number of minor updates within this appearance. Here, it is minor update 1. With minor updates, we mean changes in formatting, grammar, orthography. It can also mean adding new papers, but if these papers only confirm the score and don't change it, it will be "minor" in our book. With a change to a new appearance, the minor update will be re-set to 0.
- "(2022-11-02)" is the date of the last change – be it the initial release of the part, a minor, or a major update. The nature of the changes you may find out in the changelog next to the version number.
If an Advice, for example, has an initial release date and then just a minor update date due to link corrections, it means that – apart from correcting links – the Advice has not been updated in a major way since its initial release. Please take this into account when consulting any part of the database.
Lorem ipsum
In the fair-fish database, when you have chosen a species (either by searching in the search bar or in the species tree), the landing page is an Overview, introducing the most important information to know about the species that we have come across during our literatures search, including common names, images, distribution, habitat and growth characteristics, swimming aspects, reproduction, social behaviour but also handling details. To dive deeper, visit the Dossier where we collect all available ethological findings (and more) on the most important aspects during the life course, both biologically and concerning the habitat. In contrast to the Overview, we present the findings in more detail citing the scientific references.
Depending on whether the species is farmed or wild caught, you will be interested in different branches of the database.
Farm branch
Founded in 2013, the farm branch of the fair-fish database focuses on farmed aquatic species.
Catch branch
Founded in 2022, the catch branch of the fair-fish database focuses on wild-caught aquatic species.
The heart of the farm branch of the fair-fish database is the welfare assessment – or WelfareCheck | farm – resulting in the WelfareScore | farm for each species. The WelfareCheck | farm is a condensed assessment of the species' likelihood and potential for good welfare in aquaculture, based on welfare-related findings for 10 crucial criteria (home range, depth range, migration, reproduction, aggregation, aggression, substrate, stress, malformations, slaughter).
For those species with a Dossier, we conclude to-be-preferred farming conditions in the Advice | farm. They are not meant to be as detailed as a rearing manual but instead, challenge current farming standards and often take the form of what not to do.
In parallel to farm, the main element of the catch branch of the fair-fish database is the welfare assessment – or WelfareCheck | catch – with the WelfareScore | catch for each species caught with a specific catching method. The WelfareCheck | catch, too, is a condensed assessment of the species' likelihood and potential for good welfare – or better yet avoidance of decrease of good welfare – this time in fisheries. We base this on findings on welfare hazards in 10 steps along the catching process (prospection, setting, catching, emersion, release from gear, bycatch avoidance, sorting, discarding, storing, slaughter).
In contrast to the farm profiles, in the catch branch we assess the welfare separately for each method that the focus species is caught with. In the case of a species exclusively caught with one method, there will be one WelfareCheck, whereas in other species, there will be as many WelfareChecks as there are methods to catch the species with.
Summarising our findings of all WelfareChecks | catch for one species in Advice | catch, we conclude which catching method is the least welfare threatening for this species and which changes to the gear or the catching process will potentially result in improvements of welfare.
Try mousing over the element you are interested in - oftentimes you will find explanations this way. If not, there will be FAQ on many of the sub-pages with answers to questions that apply to the respective sub-page. If your question is not among those, contact us at ffdb@fair-fish.net.
It's right here! We decided to re-name it to fair-fish database for several reasons. The database has grown beyond dealing purely with ethology, more towards welfare in general – and so much more. Also, the partners fair-fish and FishEthoGroup decided to re-organise their partnership. While maintaining our friendship, we also desire for greater independence. So, the name "fair-fish database" establishes it as a fair-fish endeavour.